Voluntary and Elicited Discourses:
a comparison of moderator influence across sites

by

Marianne Kamp
PhD. Candidate, University of Chicago
Dept. of Near Eastern Language and Civilizations

July 29, 1997

THIS PAPER IS A DRAFT: IT IS NOT FOR DIRECT CITATION

In the project "Social Issues and Identity Formation in Estonia, Ukraine and Uzbekistan," researchers from a variety of disciplinary and professional backgrounds attempted to carry out research using an identical qualitative method across a number of very different settings, using six languages, and a large number of moderators. The focus group method was to be as uniform as possible in all settings, with the intention of establishing a basis for comparing results from qualitative research. In this paper I will try to assess the degree to which the goal of comparability was achieved, and where weaknesses in comparability lie. In this paper I will refer to about 24 of the 36 focus group transcripts which this project produced (8 from each country); this paper would certainly be more complete if I had taken time to read and analyze the structures of all of the transcripts, but, unfortunately, this is based on a rather quick overview which does not include every transcript. Direct references to the presence or lack of a certain question or feature in a focus group are certainly not intended as a critique of any of the colleagues in this study, but rather, and strictly, as examples to demonstrate what makes comparison among groups and across sites easier, and what makes such comparisons more difficult or impossible.

The assessment I will make concerning the comparability of the focus group transcripts includes attention to the following aspects: the degree of moderator control; the ways that the moderator shaped introductions to each section of the discussion; the ways that the moderator organized inputs and discussion of the two major portions--improvements and problems; the kinds of probes that the moderator used; and whether the moderator raised all of the questions that he or she was told to raise. This assessment is carried out with the hypothesis that the moderator has a significant degree of control of the process of eliciting ideas and opinions from participants. While I will concentrate on factors that will bear out this hypothesis, I will also bring into the analysis a consideration of volunteered information--that which the moderator did not prompt at all, but participants brought up on their own.

To the beginning.
To the table of contents.

Conversational Dynamics

First, in a very basic way, the dynamics of conversation can be depicted on a scale: at one end, some focus groups were completely moderator-led; at the opposite extreme, some were free-flowing, with very little moderator input. If "m" is moderator, and "p" is participant, the conversational dynamics can be abbreviated as m/p/m/p/m/p, where the moderator introduces a topic and systematically asks for a response from each member; or m/p/p/p/p, where a moderator introduces a topic, and after an initial request for a response, other members add comments, discuss and argue among themselves; or as p/p/p/p, where a participant introduces a new topic quite apart from the moderator's agenda, and other participants comment and argue. Other recurring patterns are m/p1/m/p1/m/p1, where the moderator repeatedly questions one participant, and p1/p2/p1/p2, when two participants argue their points of view at length. All of these dynamics can be found in these focus group transcripts, and in some cases, all in the same transcript. Let me state as a caveat to any further comments here that none of these dynamics is right or wrong; as David Morgan stressed in our classes on focus groups last summer, focus groups are robust. The groups discussed the same sorts of topics both when the group was heavily reliant on the moderator, and when the group carried on its own conversation with little moderator input, and even against moderator requests.

There were patterns of conversation in some groups which were not consistent with the purposes and advantages of focus group methods: in a focus group, all members should participate, and there should be a common discussion in which the participants respond not only to the moderator but also to each other. In discussing this, I will mention the names of particular groups, as examples--this does not mean that these were the only groups where these particular dynamic patterns emerged. Problems can be seen when one pattern of conversation dynamics dominated an entire transcript. The clearest example of this is the transcript for Ivankiv Ukrainian men, where most exchanges are m/p1/p2/p1/p2/p1/p2. This means that the discussion was primarily an argument between two participants, and in fact three participants in that group said almost nothing. The two participants who dominated this group had a very interesting discussion that raised many issues related to the economy, nationality, and environment, but the moderator did not intervene in their dialogue or create opportunities for others to speak. Many of the groups followed a fairly consistent m/p/m/p/p pattern, in which the moderator raised questions and solicited individual answers. For example, in Bukhara Uzbek women, the moderator established this pattern very firmly at the beginning; and participants continued to expect that the moderator would call on them to speak. In this group, the moderator’s dominant position in directing questions and sequences meant that participants often did not respond directly to the comments of others. There were several focus groups in which the dominant pattern throughout the transcript was m/p/p/p/p or p/p/p/p. This pattern, found in Tallinn Estonian women, indicated that the moderator was not very consistent in directing the conversation or in making "probe" questions; instead, participants took the conversation in their own directions, with the moderator occasionally intervening. In the ideal focus group, a balance needs to be struck between moderator-directed discourse and participant freedom to comment and exchange opinions.

To the beginning.
To the table of contents.

Eliciting and Volunteering: introductions

The brief section at the beginning of each transcript in which participants introduce themselves can serve as point of comparison between cases when the moderator prompts the participants to mention certain kinds of information, and cases when participants volunteer that information without prompting--or, alternatively, maintain silence. An examination of the occurrence of mention of family status in personal introductions will suggest the influence of a moderator’s prompting on the kinds of information that participants give. The most consistent approach to introductions was taken in all of the Uzbekistan focus groups, where moderators prompted participants to introduce themselves by talking about their work and their families. Moderators either asked for this information directly, or modeled it by introducing themselves with mention of work and family. Participants all introduced themselves with mention of their work and family status, following the moderator’s prompt.

In the Estonia focus groups, the approach to introductions was inconsistent. In one transcript (Tamsallu Estonian men) there is no evidence of any introductions at all. In a number of the groups, participants were simply asked to say a few words about themselves. In two groups (Tallinn Estonian Women and Tartu Estonian women) the moderator prompted participants to mention family, once by directly asking, and once by mentioning her marriage and daughter in her own introduction. In five groups (Narva Russian men and women, Sillimae Estonian men and women, Tartu Estonian men), moderators did not ask about families in introductions, but some of the participants mentioned their families. After one participant had mentioned family in his/her introduction, all of the following participants also mentioned family. In four groups (Tallinn Russian men, Tallinn Russian women, Tallinn Estonian men, Tamsallu Estonian women) moderators did not prompt for mention of family status in asking participants to introduce themselves, and the participants made no mention of families. These latter two clusters indicate that a moderator prompt is not a feature which absolutely controls group, but rather, influences them, and a similar influence can come from participants’ voluntary speeches. That is, in nine groups the moderator did not prompt for families, but families were mentioned anyway by participants in five of those groups. The introduction of family by one participant was enough to prompt all remaining participants to also talk about their family status. Another example of the influence of a prompt in the introduction section involves the mention of citizenship. In some of the Russian Estonia groups, though not all of them, moderators prompted participants to include in their introduction mention of where they came from. The Russian groups in Estonia which were so instructed then highlighted citizenship issues throughout their discussion, while those Russian groups which were not so instructed were also not so quick to raise citizenship issues.

To round out this picture of consistency and inconsistency in introductions, the Ukraine focus groups, like the Estonia groups, asked for participant introductions with a variety of prompts. I won’t mention every group here, but in Lviv Ukrainian men and Donetsk Russian men, the moderator did not prompt for family, and the participants introduced themselves only with reference to work and education. In Kyiv Ukrainian men, Lviv Ukrainian women and Ivankiv Ukrainian men, the moderator did not ask about families, but as soon as one participant mentioned his or her family in the introduction, all of the following participants did so as well. In Ivankiv Ukrainian women, Kyiv Ukrainian women and Donetsk Ukrainian women, moderators prompted for mention of families, and participants then mentioned their families in their introductions.

If one were to take the introductions from these focus groups and use them to suggest that women talk more about families than men do, this would be a thesis that cannot be proven. All participants talked about families when they were prompted to do so. When there were not prompted to talk about families, some women’s groups and some men’s groups talked about families anyway. When the moderator did not prompt for mention of family status, the volunteering of that information by one participant, whether male or female, was sufficient to prompt mention of family in the remaining introductions in his or her group.

Likewise, there was significant moderator impact on other topics raised in personal introductions. In some groups, moderators simply asked for introductions, and allowed participants to say one sentence each. In other groups, moderators asked follow-up questions to elicit more information. In Ivankiv Ukrainian men, the moderator followed almost each brief introduction with some additional question, concerning age, education, or work history. This gave one participant the grounds to describe his complex life history, from place of birth through recent discharge from the army. Likewise, follow-up questions in some of the Estonia Russian groups elicited stories about when participants came to Estonia, and immediately, their attitudes and concerns about citizenship. In the Uzbekistan groups none of these sorts of narratives were expressed, because the moderators specifically stated the kinds of information that they wanted in introductions, and rarely asked any follow-up questions.

To the beginning.
To the table of contents.

Discussing Improvements

Moderators on all sites were asked to use the same set of questions, and the same kinds of follow-up probes. However, in different sites, the questions and probes were handled in differing orders, and this had some bearing on the structure of the conversation, and on the amount of information that the moderator elicited. In asking about improvements, moderators were to ask what sort of improvements participants had noticed in the past ten years. Moderators were to follow this with some examination of those improvements, with questions like: has this been an improvement for everyone, or has it only affected some people? why have these changes taken place?

There was inconsistency in the way that the moderator phrased the question concerning improvements, and this had significance in shaping the kinds of improvements that participants did and did not mention. In Lviv Ukrainian men, the moderator asked in what ways political and economic changes of the last ten years improved the lives of participants and people like them. In a clarification, he stressed that these were changes that arose from socio-political changes. In a further clarification, he told participants that the changes could be on a personal level or a state level. In this group, members consistently mentioned changes on the level of society such as open borders, not having to serve in Soviet army, freedom of expression and of religion; they did not talk about intimate concerns such as marriage or the birth of a child. By contrast, in the Kyiv Ukrainian men group, the moderator asked about changes in the country which had affected the participants’ private lives in a positive way. When participants asked for clarification, the moderator stressed that these were improvements which "are tied more to yourselves, although they are related to social life". When asked to talk about their improvements, most of the Kyiv Ukrainian men mentioned events like the birth of a child, and obtaining a new apartment; these comprised about half of the improvements they mentioned, while the other half were improvements on a larger social scale, like independence, freedom of thought, peace and so forth. I chose these examples--both involving Ukrainian men-- specifically to demonstrate that the moderator’s framing of the basic question on improvements had a very significant influence over the mention of personal improvements versus social improvements. Participants in more women’s groups than men’s groups were prompted by moderators in this particular question to talk about personal improvements. In one group, Ivankiv Ukrainian women, the women only talked about personal improvements. In Tashkent Uzbek women and Ferghana Uzbek women, the moderator gave equal weight to personal and social improvements in her introduction of this topic, and the women then mentioned as many personal improvements as social improvements. In light of the fact that moderators framed this question differently in different groups, it would be difficult to prove any assertions that discourses about private and family life should be expected more in women’s groups than in men’s focus groups. When the moderator emphasized improvements in society in women’s focus groups, women were more likely than men to talk about improvements that affected children, in general, than men were. But discourses about private and family life were produced both in men’s and women’s groups when moderators specifically elicited them by stressing the personal when introducing the topic of improvements.

The structure of questioning when eliciting improvements also varied from group to group, and from country to country, and this may have had a significant influence over the relative prominence which topics received. Again, the moderators in Uzbekistan focus groups were the most consistent in their approach to eliciting participants’ mention of improvements. Moderators in these groups told participants to mark their most important improvement. They turned to the first participant, asked for his or her most important improvement, wrote it down, and then turned to the second participant, and so on. The only follow-up questions were for clarification of expression. When all of the "most important" improvements had been written, the moderator proceeded around the group, one by one, until he or she accumulated a list of all of the improvements. Rarely did the moderator ask the question "does anyone have something similar. " While participants were encouraged to mention items that were the same as those mentioned by others, the moderators put higher value on making sure that every participant had his or her turn to speak and to mention every improvement that he or she listed. In many cases, this resulted in long and varied lists of improvements. [In Uzbekistan, Uzbek and Tajik groups accumulated lists of between 8 and 18 improvements, while the Russian groups listed 6 improvements each, Karakalpak men named 5 improvements and Karakalpak women named 4 improvements.]

In the second level of questioning, consistency in the Uzbekistan groups broke down. In most groups, but not all, participants were asked to rank the improvements, voting for the one they considered most important. In many groups, moderators asked participants to explain why improvements came about, and who benefited. However, in some groups (for example Bukhara Tajik and Uzbek women), participants were asked instead to explain how they understood a particular improvement, or what affect it had on them personally. In Tashkent Uzbek women, the moderator asked each participant in what way she had personally experienced this improvement, rather than approaching the question "who benefited" in a more general way. In a number of the Uzbekistan groups, the discussion of improvements really went no further than listing and describing improvements. The moderator was in control of the structure of questioning; and in these groups the very brief mention of many topic, rather than a fuller elucidation or an open, contentious discussion, is more attributable to the moderator's influence--the follow-up probes that moderators did NOT make--than to the participants’ interest or lack thereof in the topics they raised.

In the Ukraine and Estonia focus groups, the approach to eliciting improvements was far less consistent and more eclectic. In Ivankiv Ukrainian men, the model followed was closest to the model used in the Uzbekistan focus groups: moderators asked participants to mark the most important improvement, and then proceeded around the circle one by one, eliciting improvements and assembling a list. The moderator made no follow-up probes at all, until one participant mentioned that one could now be proud to be Ukrainian, at which the moderator asked whether others concurred, sparking a discussion. In Kyiv Ukrainian men, the moderator also went around the circle assembling a list, but in this case, the moderator had not asked participants to speak briefly, or to mention their most important improvement first. In the only case of its kind, the Kyiv Ukrainian men each introduced all of their improvements in a long speech which included a full defense of the reasons that this improvement was important, and some allusion to its causes. After these speeches, the moderator made no follow-up probes at all. In Narva Russian women, the moderator did not ask women to mark their most important improvement, and assembled her list by asking one woman for an improvement, then asking if others had something similar, and then going on to the next improvement. The moderator of this group did ask follow up questions: who benefited? (but not, why did these changes take place)

In all other groups in Ukraine and Estonia, the procedure for eliciting improvements gave more emphasis to follow-up questions, and far less to assembling a full list of improvements or ranking those improvements. For example, in Lviv Ukrainian women, the moderator first elicited two improvements and asked others for similar topics. She then asked participants: why has this improved your life? After some discussion, she turned to another participant for mention of another improvement. She asked other follow-up questions after other improvements, such as "why is this important?" Participants mentioned five improvements, and the moderator had them vote on the most important. In the discussion of which was most important, the participants themselves turned the conversation to the reasons that the improvements came about, relieving the moderator of responsibility for using this as a probe question.

In most of the Estonia groups, the most consistent element in section eliciting improvements was the moderator’s use of the follow-up question, "is this improvement available to everyone?" In most of the Estonia groups, the moderator followed one of two patterns. In one model he/she elicited one improvement and asked if others had something similar and then opened discussion of this particular improvement with the follow up "does this affect everyone" before eliciting any more improvements. In the second model he/she elicited a cluster of several improvements, and then opened discussion of that group of improvements before going on to elicit more improvements for the list. Both of these patterns were followed in Ukraine as well. The extreme example of this approach can be seen in the Tartu Estonian men discussion. The first participant mentioned as an improvement that now one did not have to know the Russian language, because Estonian is the official language. The moderator followed up with a question that emphasized the problems of this change: "the rights of Estonians have increased and the rights of Russians have decreased, so you could say that this positive change does not affect everyone in Estonia." This launched a long discussion of language issues and the Estonian men’s views of Russian attitudes toward learning Estonian and becoming citizens, a topic which recurred throughout the transcript again and again. One wonders whether participants would have deemed this such a prominent issue if they had first been asked to assemble a list of their improvements and rank which one was most important. Perhaps this issue would still have occupied the majority of this focus group, but the structure of questioning certainly had some affect the amount of time spent on this topic. After this lengthy discussion, the moderator again elicited another improvement, and asked such follow-up questions as "don’t you see any problems here?," and "why has it happened?" Eventually participants named seven improvements; the moderator allowed open discussion of all, but did not ask his own follow-up questions concerning causes or who benefited about all of the topics. In this, he had good company; in almost no group was there a consistent pattern of follow-up questioning concerning improvements.

There is no single ideal structure of questioning; each structure had its advantages and disadvantages. The pattern used in the Uzbekistan groups led to the assembling of long lists of improvements, far longer than common in the other countries. This is certainly not because the groups in Uzbekistan have lives that are so much more wonderful than elsewhere; it was because the moderators were very thorough in eliciting and listing all improvements that participants had written down. However, in assembling and ranking improvements before (or in some cases, instead of) discussing and following up on improvements as they were mentioned, the Uzbekistan groups produced sections on improvements which contain many brief mentions of many topics, but very few extended discourses about any particular improvements. On the other hand, groups in Ukraine and Estonia that followed the other method of eliciting improvements--elicit one, discuss it thoroughly, and then go on--assembled less complete lists of participants improvements, with some participants even being reluctant to mention theirs. However, these groups discussed particular improvements at greater length, and with greater variety in follow-up questioning. The length of discussion of any particular issue may not have correlated with its relative importance in participants’ minds, as a group, but with their importance to one person in the group, and with the kinds of follow-up questions that the moderator chose to make, or not to make.

With all of these systematic difference, at what level is there commonality and comparability in the Improvements section of the transcripts? With some caveats, most groups did bring up and discuss improvements that affect their society. Mention of some topics as "improvements" is fairly common across all sites, and differences in emphasis from country to country and nationality to nationality have some bearing for interpreting identity formation.

To the beginning.
To the table of contents.

Discussing Problems

Most moderators--and participants--seemed to regard the discussion of problems to be more important than the discussion of improvements. Within comments on improvements in focus groups from all countries, participants repeatedly turned the conversation toward a discussion of problems. Moderators often cut short discussions of improvements, asking making few or no follow-up probes, and sometimes pointing out that their turn toward problems reflected the participants’ obvious interest.

The framing of the "problems" section was less problematic than that of the "improvements" section, in part because participants had already clarified the parameters of the question--personal or social--in the "improvements" section, and did not need to have them repeated. In a few of the groups, the question was framed very simply: name three problems or deteriorations in your life and the lives of people like you in the past 10 years. Some moderators added definitions of these changes. One of these framings provoked a minor rebellion; in Lviv Ukrainian men, the moderator asked participants to name political and economic changes that have caused difficulties. The participants in the group, who began the whole discussion with the very common assertion that there many not have been any improvements in the last ten years, now refused to find any problems. Political changes had not caused any difficulties, they asserted; there were ecological problems, perhaps, but not political problems. The moderator then reframed the question to ask, "Is there anything in your lives that has become more complicated?" At this, participants managed to think of three problems, and one or two more came up in the course of their discussion.

There were some other variations on the framing of the "problems" question which may have influenced the ways that participants thought of and discussed problems. In Bukhara Uzbek women, the moderator asked about "three difficulties which have arisen in our young country," in effect suggesting to participants that the reason for the problems could be found in the fact that Uzbekistan is new as a country. This may have contributed to the use of a comparison of a young country to a young family, which arose in several Uzbek women's groups; they used a narrative that portrays the hardships that a young couple has when moving into their own home, trying to acquire household goods and raise young children, as a way to explain and naturalize the hardships of a young country as problems which time will resolve. In Kyiv Ukrainian men, the moderator framed the "problems" question first as "three social events which had a negative influence on you and people around you," but then went on to add that this could include personal events, tragedies and dramatic events in personal life as well as social life. The first participant to respond gave a rather long speech in which he talked about unemployment, and emphasized its affect on his personal life, and then went on to his other personal problems. In Ivankiv Ukrainian men, the moderator asked participants to name problems, and added as an example, "If you think that independence was a change for the worse, write that here." Whether this provocative framing had any influence on the Ivankiv Ukrainian men focus group is difficult to assess, because the group was thoroughly dominated by two contentious men who had diametrically opposed opinions on every matter, but for the sake of comparability, the question should have been framed in a more neutral way.

As in the case of "improvements" methods for eliciting names of problems from the participants were divided into two camps, which I will call the "listing method" and the "discussion method." All of the Uzbekistan groups and a number groups from Ukraine and Estonia, including Narva Russian women, Narva Russian men, Donetsk Russian women, Donetsk Russian men, Lviv Ukrainian men (and there may be others) used the approach of telling participants to mark the most important problem, and then going one by one, naming the problems, with very limited follow-up remarks. In most of the Uzbekistan groups, moderators followed this accumulation of a list with a process of consolidating topics and voting for a group ranking of their importance. This ranking, then, established the order in which the problems were discussed. In some groups from the other countries--Donetsk Russian men, for example--the moderator had participants vote to rank the importance of problems AFTER extensive discussion of most of the problems. The other method for eliciting problems, which was used in the majority of Estonia and Ukraine groups, went as follows: the moderator elicited one problem, asked whether others had something similar, and opened a moderate to extensive discussion of this problem or cluster of problems, before going on to ask whether other participants had other problems.

Focus groups, whether they used the listing method or the mention and discuss method, elicited numerous social problems (and very few strictly personal problems). Participants in the "listing method" groups tended to generate more problems than in "discussion method" groups, but a couple of groups of each style mentioned as many as 18 or 19 problems ("discussion method"--Kyiv Ukrainian women, 17; Kyiv Ukrainian men, 18; "listing method"-- Bukhara Tajik men, 19; Tashkent Uzbek men, 18). Groups using both methods usually raised between 7 and 13 problems. In the "discussion method" groups, the topics that were mentioned first tended to receive the most discussion. In the "listing method" groups, particularly the Uzbekistan groups, topics that were ranked highest by voting received the most discussion.

To the beginning.
To the table of contents.

Follow-up Questioning

Moderator used a great variety of follow-up questions to elicit discussion about problems, and this variety itself impinges on comparability across sites. As a major caveat to moderator influence through follow-up questioning, it should be noted that in the "problems" section, moreso than in the "improvements" section, participants often took the conversation in their own directions; p/p/p/p dynamics occurred very often in the "problems" portion of the discussion. In the original planning of these discussions, it had been agreed that while moderators could use a variety of probes, there were certain ones that they should include. Two of these, "who is to blame/responsible?" and "who/which group suffers most from this problem?" are the questions which will form the basis of my comparison in this section. Did moderators use these questions, in some form? How did they use these questions? What other probes did they make?

The "discussion method" moderators favored using numerous direct follow-up questions as each topic was raised, and then asking several general follow-up questions about all the problems collectively. The "listing method" moderators usually went through problems in sequence of importance, asking all of their follow-up questions about one problem before going on to the next, not asking general follow-up questions about topics collectively. [I am dividing this discussion from the general follow-ups concerning gender and nationality which moderators were directed to raise; those will be discussed in the next section of the paper.]

The question "who is to blame, who is responsible?" was asked directly in some groups, indirectly in some groups, and in a few groups, was not asked at all. In a few groups, participants brought up the issue of blame without any prompt from the moderator. Examples taken from about ten groups will suggest the realm of possibilities in follow-up questioning related to blame.

1) In Bukhara Uzbek men, the moderator assembled a list of 18 problems, consolidated them under a few topics, had participants vote on importance, and then went though the topics one at a time. With each topic, he turned to the first participant with a series of questions: "Where did this problem come from? Is there someone who is responsible for it? Who is guilty, if anyone?" He then turned to the next participant with the same questions. He followed this round of questions with another round on the same topic, concerning who suffers most, whether the problem is improving or worsening, and who can resolve it. Following this round, he went on to the next topic, again asking each participant for his thoughts as to the reasons for the topic and whether anyone was responsible for it. Not surprisingly, participants in this group spent quite a bit of time discussing who was to blame for various problems, from "the leaders" to "the President," to "we are to blame ourselves." In most of the Uzbekistan groups, the question of blame was raised with this system, directed to each participant in follow-up questioning on each major "problem."

2) In Lviv Ukrainian men, the moderator elicited three or four problems, all related to the economy, including unemployment, low salaries, irregular salaries, economic collapse. He then asked follow-up questions about these problems, collectively, beginning with "What is the reason for these difficulties?" Participants, in answering this question, began to raise personal blame, particularly for bosses who live well, robbing their factories. The moderator later asked directly, once, "who is at fault?" which elicited a lively discussion (dynamics both m/p and p/p/p/p) about politics and politicians. Other groups used a similar pattern; in Tashkent Russian men, the moderator asked "who is responsible for these problems" as a collective follow-up; participants had a argument over whether Gorbachev or Nikolai II should be blamed. In Tartu Estonian women, the moderator made various follow-up questions to problems as women brought them up (it was a "discussion method" group) and at the end asked "who is responsible?" as a collective question concerning all the problems. Many other groups followed this pattern as well; in these groups, there are fewer mentions of blame than in the groups which followed the pattern above (no.1), simply because in the pattern 1 groups, the moderator asked directly about blame many times, and in the pattern 2 groups, the moderator asked directly about blame only once.

3) In quite a number of the focus groups, moderators asked "what is the cause of these problems" but did not directly ask "who is to blame?" In Tashkent Uzbek women, the moderator did not directly ask, "who is to blame." Participants themselves brought up blame when the moderator asked about the reasons for high costs and poor treatment in medical care; they assigned blame to corrupt doctors and to themselves for paying bribes and keeping doctors accustomed to bribe-taking. In Narva Russian women, participants dominated the "problems" section (the dynamic was mostly p/p/p/p), with the moderator asking few follow-up questions, and not directly asking "who is to blame?" At one point, a participant made a comment about leaders who had "turned away;" the moderator's follow-up to this comment, "Who turned away?" came the closest to being a probe for blame. However, the moderator immediately added a second question, about the direction of changes, and participants focused their answers on that question, rather than question "Who turned away?"

4) In Sillimae Russian men, the moderator made many follow-up questions, but never directly asked "who is to blame?" or even "what is the reason for these changes?" However, participants themselves introduced blame spontaneously, choosing this topic although the moderator had asked an entirely different question (oriented to the positive, rather than the negative) concerning what could be done to resolve the issue of factory closings. There were a number of other groups, including Tartu Estonian men, and Ivankiv Ukrainian men, in which participants raised issues of blame without any particular prompt from the moderator.

5) In Kyiv Ukrainian women, discourses about blame did not arise at all. The moderator did not ask directly, "who is to blame?" The most closely related question, which in other groups sparked discussion of blame, "What is the basic reason for these problems," elicited a discussion of causes, but no assignment of blame.

6) In two groups, Ivankiv Ukrainian men and Ivankiv Ukrainian women, moderators raised the question of blame, but did not do so during the general discussion of problems. In both of these groups, there was a section specifically concerning Chornobyl, and moderators raised the question of blame in discussion of that problem in particular. However, as mentioned above, in the Ivankiv Ukrainian men group, participants raised the issues of blame on their own, earlier, in the general discussion of problems.

The question "who suffers most" or "who is most affected by these problems" was raised in most groups, in some form. Again, the Uzbekistan groups showed the most methodological uniformity in introducing this as a follow-up probe, while in Estonia and Ukraine groups, some moderators asked this question directly, some asked it obliquely, and some did not ask it at all.

1) Tashkent Uzbek women will serve as an example of the most systematic introduction of "who suffers." In this group, the moderator introduced this question in two ways, and repeatedly. After assembling a list of problems, she took up the first problem and went around the group asking each participant in turn whether she had personally experienced this problem. This elicited some stories of personal suffering and collective suffering. Later in the discussion regarding each problem, she asked each participant to answer "who in society feels the affects of this problem most?" Participants answered with their thoughts as to whether pensioners, families with many children, or people on the state budget suffered the most--typical responses in Uzbekistan, and, with the exception of families with many children, in Estonia and Ukraine as well. In a number of the other Uzbekistan groups, the question "who suffers" was asked in the same, repetitive way. As with the question "who is to blame," the amount of time spent on "who suffers" should not be interpreted as reflecting the Uzbekistan groups' greater interest in this question, but should be recognized as a function of the moderator's system of questioning.

2) In several of the Estonia groups, moderators framed the question of "suffering more" differently, asking instead questions like, "Is this a problem for everyone?" "does this concern everyone?" and a rather opposite question that elicited the same sorts of comments, "for whom is this NOT a problem?" (Tallinn Estonian women and Tartu Estonian men). In several groups, moderators raised these questions repeatedly, eliciting comments about the relative hardships of pensioners, middle-aged people and children.

3) In some groups from all three countries, moderators raised the "who suffers more" question once, as a collective follow-up in the general discussion of problems. In some of the groups, this question elicited argument. For example, in Lviv Ukrainian men, participants argued whether pensioners or workers suffered more. Some argued that pensions were small, but they were paid, while many workers had not been paid at all for months. Similar comments arose in other groups, though most groups determined that pensioners suffered more than other categories.

4) In Tallinn Estonian men, the moderator framed questions in terms of affect, not suffering, and rather narrowly: "does everyone have opportunity?" "does the lack of social security affect the entrepreneur or the non-entrepreneur more?" Participants later discussed the suffering of groups in society, pensioners, single mothers, more generally and unprompted by the moderator, while talking about financial reform.

5) In a number of groups, the question "who suffers more" or a closely related question was not asked in relation to the general discussion of problems; it was used only in framing questions about gender and nationality. [Tashkent Russian men and women, Kyiv Ukrainian women, Ivankiv Ukrainian men and women, Kyiv Ukrainian men, Tartu Estonian women, Sillimae Russian women.] In these groups, the discussion of comparative suffering then involved only gender and nationality divisions, not class or age divisions as it did in the groups represented by patterns 1-4.

The focus here has been on two particular questions, and whether they did or did not appear as follow-ups in discussion of problems; but there were many other follow-up questions that were asked, which were extremely varied. Here, the balance between system and freedom in moderator questioning raises further issues in considering the relative depth and comparability of discussions. In general, the groups from Uzbekistan used patterns of follow-up questioning which were very systematic and consistent, rendering comparison among them much easier than comparison among most other groups. However, the emphasis on system and comparability also meant that opportunities for more particular follow-ups to interesting and possibly contentious issues were not often taken by moderators. In the Estonia and Ukraine groups, system and method are obvious in some moderators' methods, but are quite unclear in others. This makes cross-group comparisons more difficult, but also meant that moderators felt the freedom to explore in depth issues that they found interesting (and unfortunately, to ignore those that they found uninteresting).

To the beginning.
To the table of contents.

Gender, Nationality and Region

Among elements that were structured into these focus groups for the sake of comparisons across sites were questions regarding gender, nationality and regional differences. Moderators chose several different ways to frame these questions, and thus shaped the ways that participants spoke about these topics. It is important to note here that in the majority of groups, at least one of these three topics was raised by participants in an earlier section of the discussion, before the moderator gave any prompts. If the hypothesis is made that unprompted participant introduction of a topic is a good indicator of participant interest in that topic, then the frequency of these voluntary speeches may serve as another indicator of similarity and difference across sites. This section will first address participants' voluntary speeches, and then turn to the systems which moderators used to elicit discussion of the topics gender, nationality and region.

In three focus groups participants were the first to make some small allusion to gender issues before any direct questioning by the moderator. In Tallinn Estonian women, in the discussion of problems, participants brought up the hardships of rural life, and mentioned that rural women’s problems with addiction to alcohol. In Kyiv Ukrainian men, while assembling the list of problems, one man spoke specifically of the lack of state help for single mothers. While discussing improvements, one participant in Tashkent Uzbek men made particular note of women's success after independence at becoming entrepreneurs and gaining high positions. In Donetsk Russian women, a gendered topic came up during a tea-break with a free, unformatted discussion; one woman proposed that in schools, girls need to be taught various feminine skills, such as how to set a table properly and how to discern a good wine.

In most groups, comments on gender were initiated by moderator questioning. Most moderators in Ukraine and Uzbekistan focus groups, and several in Estonia framed the question on gender as a collective question concerning all of the problems: "whom do these problems affect more, men or women?" Most moderators placed this question after their general discussion of problems, but some introduced it within the middle of that discussion, in relation to a particular problem. In most groups, moderators went around the group once, eliciting answers, and possibly asking a follow-up question on why the participant held his or her opinion, if the participant had not already stated that. In these cases, answers tended to be formed as generalized representations of men's or women's character, and men's or women's usual roles. In some groups, particularly Uzbekistan groups, this question was followed with one about people or organizations that could be turned to for help with issues of concern to women.

In the Estonia groups, the approach to raising gender issues showed wide variation. In Sillimae Russian men, a participant mentioned his wife's efforts to study Estonian while discussing "improvements," and the moderator followed his comment with a question on what constituted a good job and a good salary for women in Sillimae. In Sillimae Russian women, the moderator brought up gender comparisons after a participant's comment about the local vocational school. The moderator asked whether mainly boys studied at the school, which led into a brief discussion of women's job opportunities, and women going abroad for work. In Narva Russian women, the moderator framed the gender question very differently: "If men were here, do you think they would say these problems are the same for them?" In Tartu and Tallinn Estonian men, the question was framed the same way, regarding women. While this framing elicited some generalized representations of women's character and men's character and roles, it also elicited more precise remarks concerning which problems affected men more than women and vice versa.

Participant introduction of comments related to nationality, citizenship and language issues, without prompting from moderators, occurred frequently. In most of the Estonia focus groups, some discussion concerning either language issues or citizenship issues arose early in the conversations of both Estonians and Russians. In some cases, as mentioned above (under Introductions) moderators were involved in eliciting these topics by mention of place of birth or where one came from. In Ukraine, participants in one group, Ivankiv Ukrainian men, made very extensive and unsolicited comments concerning language policy and discrimination. In Kyiv Ukrainian women, participants mentioned mixed ethnic backgrounds in their introductions. In Donetsk Russian women, participants brought up language policies in follow-up questioning concerning general problems. In Kyiv Ukrainian men, ethnicity issues were raised in "improvements" in relation to language policies. In Uzbekistan, both Tashkent Russian men and women made comments about ethnic differences between Russians and Uzbeks before any moderator prompt, while in Tashkent Uzbek women, participants mentioned that they had greater trust in Russian nurses than Uzbek nurses while discussing problems in health care.

Across the three countries, moderators chose several ways of framing their discussion of problems in relation to nationality. In the Uzbekistan groups, moderators in some groups asked whether different nationalities experienced these problems differently. Others prefaced that question with a remark about the multi-national nature of Uzbekistan: among the 120 or so nationalities, is there any that is more affected by these problems? is there any that is less affected? In one group, Bukhara Tajik women, the moderator referred specifically to Uzbeks, Tajiks and Russians in framing her question. When participants replied that problems affected all equally, she added a follow-up, asking whether Tajik-speaking people had difficulty, since Uzbek had become the language of state. In an interesting elision of Uzbek-Tajik difference, the women emphasized Russian difficulty with the state language policy.

In Ukraine, the Lviv and Donetsk moderators framed their nationality question with explicit reference to only Ukrainians and Russians: "we have two main nationalities, Ukrainians and Russians," followed by a question concerning whether one of them suffered more. In the other Ukraine groups, moderators framed the question with allusions to Ukraine as a multi-national country, or with mentions of Russians, Ukrainians, Jews and other groups. In both framings, this question elicited a few comments denying difference and asserting that Slavs are Slavs, while other comments focused on Russian and Ukrainian difference.

In Estonia, moderators also chose several ways of framing their question concerning nationality. In one group, Sillimae Russian women, participants talked at length about language and citizenship issues, so that the moderator never introduced a general probe about nationality, but did ask follow-up questions to differentiate language and citizenship issues. One participant turned the table on the moderator by asking whether Estonians were saying the same things that Russians in Sillimae were saying. In Sillimae Russian men, the moderator asked the rather standard question for this study, "do the problems that we are discussing now influence the lives of people of different nationalities in different ways?" In Narva Russian women, the moderator asked participants, "if the same question (about problems) were put to an Estonian group, what problems would they list?" A similar frame was used in Tartu and Tallinn Estonian men. In Narva Russian men, the moderator placed his first nationality probe within the general discussion of problems, asking in relation to the "feeling of uncertainty" whether it is equal among the nationalities in Estonia.

Questioning on regional difference was very inconsistent. In quite a number of groups, regional difference did not come up in the conversation, and the moderator put no follow-up questions concerning region to the focus group. [Tashkent Russian Women, Tashkent Uzbek women--although one participant mentioned rural-urban difference, Tartu Estonian men, Ivankiv Ukrainian men] In Uzbekistan, two groups, Tashkent Uzbek men and Bukhara Uzbek women, brought up regional difference before the moderator asked about it, mentioning the varying availability of food products in different provinces in relation to high prices and inflation. In Estonia, participants in Narva Russian women volunteered comments comparing unemployment in Narva and in southern Estonia.

The framing of regional questions included a few main variations. In Lviv Ukrainian men, the moderator first asked about improvements particular to Lviv region, and then went on to Lviv's problems, and finally to a comparison with other regions. In other groups, such as Bukhara Uzbek women and Tallinn Estonian men, moderators began with a question about problems particular to that region or city, and then went on to comparisons with other regions. In many groups, moderators framed the question more generally, asking whether some regions were more affected by problems than others. In many of the Ukraine groups, participants alluded to differences between Eastern and Western Ukraine, and moderators introduced regional issues in relation to environmental issues, asking about Chornobyl and its relative affects on different regions.

To the beginning.
To the table of contents.

Conclusion

What basis is there for comparison across sites in this project? In making comparisons, researchers should be aware of the variations in method and in framing questions from country to country and from focus group to focus group. In most cases, moderators framed the questions, and exercised control over the length and relative prominence of discussion on particular topics through the use of follow-up questions--and their lack. Participants were also agents in discussion, most obviously in voluntary speeches, but also by making statements about some topics and staying silent on others. In a few cases participants exercised as much control over the group as the moderator did, continuing to discuss a particular subject of their choosing even when the moderator tried to turn the conversation to a new topic, and refusing to give the floor to another participant when asked. Because of these factors, variations in questions asked, and variations in conversation dynamics, an analysis of the results that attempts to build significant arguments by comparing the amount of space that groups gave to different topics would have a weak foundation. Time spent on a particular topic may indeed indicate its relative importance to the participants, or relative significance as a defining issue related to identity formation, but that would need to be corroborated using other analytical methods that take into consideration the structures of conversation in these focus groups, and, most important, the actual content of comments, regardless of length and prominence.


Return to | Table of Contents | Workshop and Papers | FSU Grant Homepage | CREES Homepage